Somatic distance, trust and trade

AuthorJacques Melitz,Farid Toubal
Date01 August 2019
Published date01 August 2019
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/roie.12397
786
|
© 2019 John Wiley & Sons Ltd wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/roie Rev Int Econ. 2019;27:786–802.
1
|
INTRODUCTION
In sociology, homophily is taken as an established fact: individuals tend to associate and bond with
similar others, as in the proverb “birds of a feather flock together.” Also, “Homophily in race and
ethnicity creates the strongest divides in our personal environments” (McPherson, Smith‐Lovin, &
James Cook, 2001, p. 415). The authors of this oft‐cited article go on: “Age, religion, education, occu-
pation, and gender [follow] in roughly that order.” Indeed, discrimination based on race and ethnicity
is readily apparent in economics too, especially in labor studies (see Bertrand & Mullainathan, 2004,
and Edo, Jacquemet, & Yannelis, 2017, concerning job applications; Lang & Lehmann, 2012, and
Borowczyk‐Martins, Bradley, & Tarasonis, 2017, concerning wage earnings and employment). Yet in
studies of bilateral trade between countries, homophily is mostly absent from the list of determinants.
It emerged only recently in a highly influential article by Guiso, Sapienza, and Zingales (2009) (here-
after GSZ). Since they wrote, two works have picked up on the theme: Spring and Grossmann (2016)
(in a critical spirit) and Yu et al. (2015). GSZ introduced somatic distance or difference in physical
appearance based on an Italian source: a work by Biasutti (1954, first edition) in four volumes, which
Received: 9 October 2018
|
Revised: 27 December 2018
|
Accepted: 4 January 2019
DOI: 10.1111/roie.12397
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Somatic distance, trust and trade
JacquesMelitz1
|
FaridToubal2
1ENSAE and CEPII, Paris
2ENS de Paris‐Saclay, CREST and CEPII
Correspondence
Jacques Melitz, ENSAE and CEPII,
20 Avenue de Segur, Paris, 75007.
Email: j.melitz@hw.ac.uk
Abstract
Somatic distance, or differences in physical appearance,
proves to be extremely important in the gravity model of
bilateral trade in conformity with results in other areas of
economics and outside in the social sciences. This is also
true independently of survey evidence about bilateral trust.
These findings are obtained in a sample of the 15 members
of the European Economic Area in 1996. Robustness tests
also show that somatic distance, as well as co‐ancestry, has
a more reliable influence on bilateral trade than the other
customary cultural variables. The article finally discusses
the interpretation and breadth of application of these
results.
JEL CLASSIFICATION
F10, F40, Z10
|
787
MELITZ and TOUBaL
summarizes and extends a huge literature on racial differences in physical anthropology. However,
while making use of Biasutti, GSZ and the two aforementioned studies, subordinate the whole issue of
somatic distance by treating it as affecting bilateral trade essentially via trust: that is, as an instrument
for trust in an instrumental‐variable‐regression interpretation.1 In this contribution, we shall instead
introduce somatic distance as a direct influence on bilateral trade right from the start and show that it
belongs there.
In his path‐breaking work on The Economics of Discrimination, Becker (1957) modeled discrimi-
nation in the labor market as founded on the distaste of employers for interacting with certain groups
of workers. His view has undergone subsequent modification, with allowances for the significance of
beliefs about other people and some rational elements in labor market discrimination (for example,
Phelps, 1972; Arrow, 1973). But as the literature in the preceding paragraph is witness, his basic posi-
tion stands: racial prejudice as such retains a firm position in explanations of racial discrimination. It
is clear that such prejudice partly stems from and partly breeds distrust. But there is no precedent for
viewing the prejudice and, broadly, somatic distance in general, as impinging on international trade
strictly through the filter of trust. No one would argue that sex discrimination is only a matter of trust.
There should be little question either that discrimination based on physical appearance is not strictly
a matter of trust. In the specific case of international trade, it is standard, since Armington (1969), to
allow that national preferences for different trade partners may intervene in explaining bilateral trade.
Should somatic distance be one of the reasons for these national preferences, personal affinities could
well be the source, independently of trust. The mechanism could take several forms. For example, an
increase in the number of foreign markets to which national firms export raises their fixed costs and
necessitates choices. In making these choices, “animal spirits” may operate and thus explain why
somatic distance enters. As another example, exporters of consumption goods might find that their
wares have more appeal to foreigners who resemble them, partly because of similar tastes, but partly
also because of a preference for associating with them in commerce.
Once we admit that somatic distance has a direct place in a gravity equation for bilateral trade, the
variable emerges as highly significant. It remains so in the presence of other cultural factors, reflecting
language, religion, law, co‐ancestry, and the history of wars, as well as sample evidence from ques-
tionnaires about trust. Indeed, it is more robust than the rest. Trust, based on questionnaire evidence,
and the history of wars never matter. Since immigrants are particularly important in studying cultural
interactions, we introduce them. This has a seriously damaging effect on two important cultural vari-
ables, same legal origin and common religion. Adding a population‐weighted measure of physical
distance in the presence of immigrants notably reduces the significance of Common native language
too. Somatic distance is hardly affected throughout. Somatic distance even outperforms co‐ancestry
though this last variable also holds up well in a new guise.
All these results occur in a European sample close to GSZ’s. There are two strong reasons for stick-
ing close to this sample. The more important one is that we want to control for trust and we know no
reasonable alternative to GSZ’s measure. But secondly, even if we were to drop GSZ’s trust variable,
we could not extend the analysis very far, only to the rest of Europe outside the European Economic
Area (EEA) in 1996, since Biasutti’s data for somatic distance permits going no further. This would
essentially add Eastern Europe and Switzerland.
As indicated, both somatic distance and co‐ancestry, or two different aspects of genetic distance,
appear important. The two variables matter singly and jointly. Yet our emphasis will be solely on so-
matic distance, essentially because of its wrongful neglect and its confusion with trust.
The next section offers the test evidence, the following one provides robustness tests concerning
the significance of somatic distance, and the last one engages in general discussion and interpretation.

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT