Negativity Bias in Attention Allocation: Retail Investors’ Reaction to Stock Returns

DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/irfi.12180
AuthorTomas Reyes
Date01 March 2019
Published date01 March 2019
Negativity Bias in Attention
Allocation: Retail Investors
Reaction to Stock Returns*
TOMAS REYES
Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, Ponticia Universidad Católica de
Chile, Santiago, Chile
ABSTRACT
We argue that negative stock market performance attracts more attention
from retail investors than comparable positive performance. Specically, we
test and conrm the hypothesis that retail investors pay more attention to
negative extreme returns than positive ones. We present a measure of atten-
tion at the aggregate and company-specic levels using Googles internet
search volume indexes. These measures correlate with, but are different from,
existing proxies of attention. Our empirical results strongly support the posi-
tion that investors display a negativity bias in attention allocation with
respect to extreme stock returns. Across all specications, lagged negative
extreme returns are stronger predictors of high attention at the individual-
stock and stock market levels than positive ones.
JEL Codes: G02; G14; D19
Accepted: 18 December 2017
I. INTRODUCTION
Psychology research supports the notion that bad is stronger than good. Bau-
meister et al. (2001) argue that in most situations, negative events will produce
larger, more consistent or more intense consequences than positive events of
comparable magnitude. Anecdotally, human beings usually ask to hear the bad
news rst, and bad news sells more newspapers. Testing whether attentional
resources are automatically directed away from the current task when inessen-
tial good or bad traits are present, Pratto and John (1991) nd that a bad extra-
neous stimulus attracts more attention in an automatic and nonintentional
* The author is grateful to Stefano DellaVigna, Simon Gervais, Isaac Hacamo, Ulrike Malmendier,
Thomas Mertens, Atif Mian, Terrance Odean, Richard Stanton, Adam Szeidl, Paul Tetlock, Hal Var-
ian, and Wei Xiong for helpful comments. He also acknowledges nancial support from Fondecyt
Iniciación (No. 11130647), Fondecyt Regular (Nos. 1160048 and 1171894), and Nucleo Milenio
Research Center for Entrepreneurial Strategy under Uncertainty (No. NS130028). Part of the work
was completed while the author was at UC Berkeley.
© 2018 International Review of Finance Ltd. 2018
International Review of Finance, 19:1, 2019: pp. 155189
DOI: 10.1111/ir.12180
fashion than a good stimulus. And in a study of how long positive or negative
everyday events continue to impact a persons mood, Sheldon et al. (1996) con-
clude that negative information takes longer to process and contributes more to
the creation of impressions than positive information.
In this paper, we relate the negativepositive attention asymmetry found in
psychology to stock market behavior. We argue that negative stock market per-
formance draws more attention than comparable positive performance. Speci-
cally, we measure performance using stock returns and test the hypothesis that
retail investors pay more attention to extreme negative returns than to extreme
positive ones.
There is an inherent challenge in directly measuring attention and its alloca-
tion across tasks. We measure attention in the stock market using Google Trends,
which provides a search volume tool that is a powerful proxy of attention for
two reasons. First, it is common for internet users to search for information using
Google, so the results of Google Trends are truly representative of their interest
in a topic. Additionally, since searching for a term on Google obviously requires
paying attention to it, search volume from Google Trends is a better proxy for
attention than alternative instruments used in the literature previously.
The results of Googles search volume tool are expressed in terms of the
search volume index (SVI). The SVI for a search term is the percentage of
searches for that term throughout 1 week within a geographical region, scaled
by its time-series maximum. Data are available from January 2004 for most
common terms used in Google searches.
Multiple authors have used this data in different areas for modeling and pre-
diction. Ginsberg et al. (2008) employ Googles indexes to predict u outbreaks
more efciently than the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC).
Choi and Varian (2012) use it to predict sales and tourism. Da et al. (2011) pro-
vide support for the Barber and Odean (2008)s price pressure hypothesis using
search data on ticker symbols. Da et al. (2015) use search volume to measure
investor sentiment and show that decreases in search volume are correlated
with price increases, which then reverse in the short term. Campos et al. (2017)
use search volume to model and predict the oils VIX, nding that search data
signicantly increases the returns of volatility-exposed portfolios.
In this paper, we use three aggregate measures of investor attention to the
stock market based on Google search volume data. The rst measure is Atten-
tion to the Stock Market (StockMarket), dened as the sum of the SVI values for
a series of terms such as stock marketor best stocksthat investors typically
search when seeking general information about the whole stock market. The
second measure, attention from potential market entrants (OnlineTrading), is
the sum of the SVI values for a series of terms such as online trading,”“online
brokerage account,or best brokerage account,and captures the tendency of
potential investors to enter the market, that is, to search for information on
opening a brokerage account. Finally, attention from existing investors (Etrade)
is a measure of retail investors who already have a brokerage account and use
Google to access its webpage and log in. It is dened as the sum of the SVI
© 2018 International Review of Finance Ltd. 2018156
International Review of Finance
values for a series of terms such as etradeor ameritrade.In addition to the
previous three indicators, we implement a measure of attention at the stock
level. Following Da et al. (2011) we use the SVI for the ticker symbols of large
companies in the S&P 500 (e.g., YHOOfor Yahoo or WMTfor WalMart).
After constructing these measures, we proceed as follows. First, we study
what drives SVI and how it relates to other indirect proxies for attention. We
nd that aggregate US-level SVI measures have positive contemporaneous and
lagged correlations with trading volume and volatility. When we explore the
relationship between lagged returns and attention, we nd that the measures
StockMarket and OnlineTrading display the greatest amount of attention to
extreme positive and negative returns. More importantly, lagged negative
extreme returns are stronger predictors of attention in the stock market than
positive extreme returns.
Second, we get SVI data from Google for each US state and construct in-state
versions of our attention variables. We then sort the companies by state using
location codes from Compustat. For each state and week, we construct a portfo-
lio of in-state rms with a high-market capitalization. As with the US-level
results, we nd that individual investors show a negativity bias and pay more
attention to negative than positive extreme returns.
Third, we focus on attention to specic stocks and its relationship to individ-
ual stock returns. We use SVI for ticker symbols and stock-level market data to
create a panel with the 100 largest companies in the S&P 500 for which we
have complete data. Here again we nd patterns supporting a negativity bias,
even after controlling for other proxies of attention.
Overall, our empirical results strongly support the view that investors display
a negativity bias in attention allocation with respect to extreme stock returns.
Across all specications, lagged negative extreme returns are stronger predictors
of retail investorsattention than positive extreme returns. To the best of our
knowledge, this is the rst time such a pattern has been documented for US
investors.
We perform several robustness checks. First, we show that the negativity bias
is present in subsamples of companies with both low- and high-institutional
ownership; however, this bias is amplied among companies with a larger frac-
tion of individual investors. This is consistent with previous ndings that sup-
port SVI as a proxy for attention from individual investors (Da et al. 2011).
Second, when splitting the sample between groups of stocks with low and high
(lagged) abnormal trading volume, we nd that investors pay more attention to
extreme negative returns than positive ones in both subgroups; however, they
display a greater amount of sensitivity to extreme returns that are accompanied
by high volume. Third, we nd no evidence to support the possibility that our
results are driven by a negative media bias (that is, more media attention to
negative events than positive ones). Finally, we rule out the possibility that neg-
ative returns are stronger predictors of attention simply because they are more
unusual or because negative and positive returns are not symmetrical events to
stockholders.
© 2018 International Review of Finance Ltd. 2018 157
Negativity Bias in Attention Allocation

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT