Effect of religious priming in prosocial and destructive behaviour

DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0106.12293
AuthorElizabeth Brown,Huan Xie,Jipeng Zhang
Published date01 February 2020
Date01 February 2020
ORIGINAL MANUSCRIPT
Effect of religious priming in prosocial
and destructive behaviour
Jipeng Zhang
1
| Elizabeth Brown
2
| Huan Xie
2
1
Research Institute of Economics and
Management, Southwestern University of Finance
and Economics, Chengdu, China
2
Department of Economics, Concordia University,
Quebec, Canada
Correspondence
Jipeng Zhang, Research Institute of
Economics and Management, Southwestern
University of Finance and Economics,
55 Guanghuacun Street, Chengdu, Sichuan
Province, China 610074.
Email: jpzhang@swufe.edu.cn
Funding information
FQRSC, Grant/Award Number: 2010-NP-133118
Abstract
In this paper, we study the behavioural impact of religious
priming by showing participants religious words in a
scrambled sentence task before a dictator game and a joy-
of-destruction game. We also elicited data on individual
religiosity and religious affiliation using a questionnaire.
Priming religious words significantly increased prosocial
behaviour in the dictator game, and the effect was espe-
cially striking among those reporting no religion, atheists
and agnostics. The religious prime has no significant
effect in mitigating destructive behaviour or own expecta-
tions of the others destruction choice, but both destructive
behaviour and expectations correlate positively with the
multi-dimensional religiosity measure.
KEYWORDS
dictator game, joy-of-destruction game, prosocial
behaviour, religious priming
1|INTRODUCTION
Religion has been linked to a range of social and economic decisions as far back as Smith (1776). In
modern research, economists have investigated the relationship between religion and income
(Crabtree, 2010), GDP growth (Barro & McCleary, 2003), labour supply (Granger & Price, 2007)
and other variables. McCleary and Barro (2006) discuss religions two-way interaction with the polit-
ical economy, both as a dependent variable and an independent variable. The present paper contrib-
utes to the economics research on religion and prosocial behaviour by soliciting donations to a real
charity, adding the dimension of antisocial behaviour, and using a scrambled sentence task to prime
participants with religious words. Moreover, we collected information on individual religiosity and
religious affiliation in a questionnaire to evaluate the differentiated priming effects among different
religious groups.
Received: 2 August 2017 Revised: 28 July 2018 Accepted: 12 January 2019
DOI: 10.1111/1468-0106.12293
Pac Econ Rev. 2020;25:4768. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/paer © 2019 John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd 47
In the previous literature, researchers found that religions tend to espouse prosocial values such
as charity, forgiveness, honesty and tolerance. Theories of religion generally posit that religion fosters
prosocial behaviour (Norenzayan & Shariff, 2008).
1
Many empirical researches have studied the
effects of religious priming on prosocial behaviour (see also the recent meta-analyses by Shariff, Wil-
lard, Andersen, and Norenzayan (2015) and by van Elk et al. (2015)).
2
Different from most previous
studies, subjects in our study do not play a standard dictator game, but an Eckel and Grossman
(1996) dictator game, in which they choose an amount to be donated to a charity instead of an anony-
mous stranger. One research question in our study is to investigate whether religious priming would
affect the decision to donate for a good cause differently compared to the decision to allocate money
to a stranger.
Religions tend to discourage antisocial behaviours such as deception and spite. There is an eco-
nomics literature on negative behaviours (e.g. negative reciprocity, defection in the prisoner dilemma,
free-riding in public goods games and spiteful behaviour in ultimatum games). However, the study of
direct antisocial behaviour that does not arise from retaliation against other negative behaviour (per-
ceived or otherwise) is limited. Abbink and Sadrieh (2009) introduced the joy-of-destruction game in
which two players each receive an endowment and simultaneously decide on how much of the other
players endowment to destroy. Again in the joy-of-destruction game, Abbink and Herrmann (2011)
find that approximately one-quarter of participants will destroy half their anonymous partners
endowment when the source of the destruction is hidden. To our knowledge, there has not been any
experimental research on the subject of proactive antisocial behaviour and religiosity or religious
priming.
Our study also adjoins the experimental economics literature that has studied the relationship
between self-reported measures of religiosity and prosocial behaviour in various economic games.
Most of these studies have found no significant relationship between religiosity and prosocial behav-
iour.
3
One novel part of our study is that we incorporate the self-reported religiosity measures into
the experimental analysis on the priming effects in prosocial and destructive behaviour, so that we
can shed light on the possible differentiated impact of religious priming on different religious groups
categorized by self-reported religiosity measures.
In summary, we present an experimental study in which half of our participants were primed with
religious words before they made a dictator game and a joy-of-destruction game decision. We
observed that priming with religious words increased prosocial behaviour in the dictator game but
1
An interesting recent experimental study conducted in a Muslim country by Haruvy, Ioannou, and Golshirazi (2018) shows
that observant workers who are in the midst of fasting during Ramadan are far more generous to recipients than workers who
have had their evening meal, but it is reversed when decision-makers are outside Ramadan.
2
Randolph-Seng and Nielsen (2007) found that participants cheated significantly less in an honesty task when they had been
primed with religious words. Pichon, Boccato, and Saroglou (2007) found that priming participants with religious words led to
them taking more charity pamphlets when leaving an experimental session. Malhotra (2010) shows that religious participants
were more likely to keep bidding on Sundays, and Malhotra concludes that Sunday served as a prime that activated religious
norms in religious subjects. Benjamin, Choi, and Fisher (2016) found no significant effect of religious priming on dictator
game giving. Lambarraa and Riener (2012) found that Moroccan subjects gave more in a dictator game when the experiment
was administered in Arabic versus French; the Arabic language served as a prime of Arabic and Muslim identity. Shariff and
Norenzayan (2007) found that priming people with religious words made them more generous in the dictator game.
3
Eckel and Grossman (1996) found no significant difference between religious and non-religious people in the amount of giv-
ing to secular charities. Similar results are reported by Anderson and Mellor (2009) and Ahmed and Salas (2011) for the public
goods game. Using a multi-dimensional measure of religiosity, Tan (2006) finds no influence of religiosity on pro-sociality
measured by dictator and ultimatum games because of independent religiosity dimensions producing counter-working effects
that cancel each other out. In a trust game that reveals a responders religiosity score, Tan and Vogel (2008) find that both a
proposers trust and a responders trustworthiness increase with the responders religiosity.
48 ZHANG ET AL.

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT